Health Technologies

‘Smart’ drugs can ‘decrease productivity in neurotypical students’

Neurotypical workers and students taking cognitive enhancers or ‘smart’ drugs may actually be inhibiting their performance and productivity, new research from the University of Cambridge and the University of Melbourne has found.

Drugs such as methylphenidate, sold under the brand name Ritalin among others, are commonly prescribed for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), but are also taken by people without a diagnosis, in the belief that the drugs will enhance focus and cognitive performance.

In four double-blinded, randomised trials in Melbourne, each a week apart, the same 40 healthy participants took either methylphenidate, modafinil, dextroamphetamine or a placebo.

They were then assessed on how they performed in a test designed to model the complex decision-making and problem-solving present in our everyday lives.

While previous studies into the effects of smart drugs have used simpler cognitive tasks targeting memory or attention, the Melbourne trial involved more computationally complex activities that better simulate the difficult nature of tasks people encounter in everyday life.

Participants were asked to complete an exercise known as the Knapsack Optimisation Problem – or ‘knapsack task’ – where they were given a virtual knapsack with a set capacity, and a selection of items of different weights and values.

The participants had to work out how to best allocate items to the bag, to maximise the overall value of its contents.

Overall, participants taking the drugs saw small decreases in accuracy and efficiency, alongside large increases in time and effort, relative to their results when not taking the drugs.

For example, when given methylphenidate – often used to treat ADHD in children, but increasingly taken by college students cramming for exams – participants took around 50 per cent longer on average to complete the knapsack problem as when they were given a placebo.

In addition, the participants who performed at a higher level in the placebo condition compared to the rest of the group tended to show a bigger decrease in performance and productivity after receiving a drug.

In terms of “productivity”, for example – the level of progress per item moved in or out of the knapsack – the participants in the top 25 per cent under a placebo regularly ended up in the bottom 25 per cent under methylphenidate.

By contrast, participants who had a lower performance in a placebo condition only very occasionally exhibited a small improvement after taking a drug.

Professor Peter Bossaerts, Leverhulme International Professor of Neuroeonomics at the University of Cambridge, said:

“Our results suggest that these drugs don’t actually make you ‘smarter.

“Because of the dopamine the drugs induce, we expected to see increased motivation, and they do motivate one to try harder.

“However, we discovered that this exertion caused more erratic thinking — in ways that we could make precise because the knapsack task had been widely studied in computer science.

“Performance did not generally increase, so questions remain about how the drugs are affecting people’s minds and their decision making.”

Dr Elizabeth Bowman researcher at the Centre for Brain, Mind and Markets at the University of Melbourne and lead author of the study, added:

“Our research shows drugs that are expected to improve cognitive performance in patients may actually be leading to healthy users working harder while producing a lower quality of work in a longer amount of time.”

You may also like

Health Technologies

Accelerating Strategies Around Internet of Medical Things Devices

  • December 22, 2022
IoMT Device Integration with the Electronic Health Record Is Growing By their nature, IoMT devices are integrated into healthcare organizations’
Health Technologies

3 Health Tech Trends to Watch in 2023

Highmark Health also uses network access control technology to ensure computers are registered and allowed to join the network. The